Cognitive Development: Jean Piaget
Piaget and his family |
Jean Piaget
(1896–1980) was known best for his work in studying the cognitive development
of children and adolescents. Piaget
hypothesized that children’s cognitive progress through four critical stages in
an orderly sequence manner. The four stages correspond with the age of the
child, which include the sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete
operational and formal operational stages.
Piaget
described and explained cognitive development using concepts like schema,
assimilation, accommodation and equilibration. Piaget believed that
accommodating and assimilating information develop cognitive operations.
Knowledge is invented and reinvented by the means of these two important
processes as the child develops and constantly interacts with the world around
him.
Piaget's concept of cognitive development |
Schema is defined as a pattern of action or a mental
structure that is involved in acquiring or organizing knowledge. In other
words, schema is a unit of knowledge or understanding that helps us to organize
and simplify our knowledge of the world around us. Schema can be applied to
different things like objects, animals, actions, beliefs or ideas. It is worth
to note that schemas are uniquely varied from person to person as a result of
different background and experiences. According to Piaget, schemas are evolving
structures in a sense that they are continuously becoming more refined and
sophisticated as the child develops. Theoretically, schemas are developed and
modified through continuous interweaving processes of assimilation and
accommodation.
Assimilation
refers to the process by which someone uses existing schemas to deal with new
information and experiences. On the other hand, accommodation is the process of
adjusting or modifying schemas to fit new information and experiences. The process
of creating an equilibrium or balance between assimilation and accommodation is
termed as equilibration. Piaget suggested that equilibration takes place in
three phases. First children are satisfied with their mode of thought and
therefore are in a state of equilibrium. Then, they become aware of the
shortcomings in their existing thinking and are dissatisfied (i.e., are in a
state of disequilibration and
experience cognitive conflict). Lastly, they adopt a more sophisticated mode of
thought that eliminates the shortcomings of the old one (i.e., reach a more
stable equilibrium).
For example,
because there is a cat adopted in his house, a child develops a schema that cat
is a four-legged animal, furry, and meowing. The child then sees a cat
somewhere else and points at it and asks an adult, “cat?” and the adult
confirms his query. So here, seeing a cat and correctly labeling it ‘cat’ is a
case of assimilating the animal into the child’s cat schema. The child is
satisfied and in a state of equilibrium because everything seems to fit his cat
schema. Later, he sees an animal that is four-legged and furry, but instead of
meowing, this animal barks. This leads to disequilibrium because the new
information (barking) does not fit his cat schema and thus causing cognitive
conflict to the child. Accordingly, when the child labels that dog as a ‘cat’, an
adult then tells him, “It isn’t a cat, it is a dog”. So now, the child
accommodates this new information and learns, “Not all four-legged animals are
cats, those that barks are dogs”. This demonstrates how the child uses
equilibration (through accommodation) and incorporates the term ‘dog’ into his
dog schema, putting him into a state of equilibrium again.
Infancy: The Sensorimotor Stage
The first stage
of Piaget’s theory is the sensorimotor stage, which lasts from birth to age 2.
It is called sensorimotor because Piaget believed that infants in this period
operate cognitively based almost entirely on their sensory experiences (i.e.,
the things an infant sees, hears, smells, tastes, and feels) and motor reflexes
(e.g. grasping reflex, sucking reflex, and rooting reflex). There are 6
substages under this stage and each substages have their own milestones. The
most important achievement during this stage is to acquire the object
permanence schema, which refers to the understanding that an object still
exists even when it disappears from sight or other senses. For instance,
imagine a game of peek-a-boo. A very young infant will believe that the other
person or object has actually vanished and will act shocked or puzzled when the
object reappears. On the contrary, older infants who understand object
permanence will realize that the person or object continues to exist even when
unseen.
i.
Substage 1 -
Newborn reflexes (0-1 month)
During the
earliest part of the sensorimotor stage, inborn reflexes are the first schemas
that infant have and use to interact with the environment. Reflexive responses
such as rooting, sucking, and grasping are how an infant spontaneously responds
(assimilates) to external stimuli and does modification (accommodation)
accordingly. Take for example the sucking reflex, when an object (e.g., nipple)
is placed in infant’s mouth, he will suck that object because it stimulates his
sucking reflex. Accordingly, infants will modify (accommodate) the sucking
patterns to the shape of the nipple and the rate of flow of fluid.
ii. Substage 2 - Primary circular
reactions (1-4 months)
In primary
circular reactions, an infant develops the ability to coordinate various
schemas into complex systems, meaning that he can now look, grasp, and suck at
the same time. Infants gain a measure of control over their bodies, as they are
able to combine them into more complex actions. Infants also begin to show
primary circular reactions, involving their own bodies (e.g., thumb sucking). A
circular reaction is a behavior that produces an interesting event (initially
by accident) and so is repeated. For example, consider an infant who is moving
her hands and arms around her head in a random way. Accidentally, her thumb may
brush her lips and end up in her mouth, eliciting a sucking reaction. Infant
may find this as an “interesting” result, thus will repeat it, continuing to
suck the thumb. From the repetition or practice, the “thumb-sucking schema”
becomes more refined.
iii. Substage 3 - Secondary circular
reactions (4-8 months)
Secondary
circular reactions aim at actions that have effect on the external environment,
as opposed to the primary circular reactions, which focus on actions of the
infant’s own body. An infant in this stage may accidentally cause something
interesting in the environment to happen and then seek to repeat this
interesting event. The interesting events in this case are located in the
external world, whereas in primary circular reactions the interesting events
are occurring within the infant’s body. For example, an infant who accidentally
shakes a rattle will keep shaking it because of the pleasure of hearing the
sound it produces.
iv. Substage 4 - Coordination of
secondary circular reactions (8-12 months)
In the fourth
stage, infants are able to coordinate schemes (combine several actions) to
achieve specific goal. For example, an infant may push away an object to grasp
a toy behind it. An infant in this stage can also now imitate many gestures and
sounds from the observed behaviors of others. Most importantly, infants begin
to develop object permanence during this stage. For example, an infant learns
that even though his mother leaves the room, she has not ceased to exist and
similarly, a ball does not disappear because a cloth is placed over it.
However, infants still show A-not-B error, which occurs when infants make the
mistake of selecting the familiar hiding place (A) rather than the new hiding
place (B).
v.
Substage 5 -
Tertiary circular reactions (4-8 months)
Similar to
primary and secondary circular reactions, a tertiary circular reaction begins
when some actions accidentally lead to interesting sensory consequences. But
rather than just repeating the same behavior, the infant explores new
possibilities and varies it in a purposeful, trial-and-error way to find out
the consequences. For example, an infant may try out different sounds or
actions as a way of getting attention from a caregiver.
vi. Substage 6 - Beginning of mental
representation
Finally, there
is the beginning of mental representation in substage six, which is the ability
to visualize things that are not physically present and hold an image in mind
longer. Because they can symbolically imagine things that cannot be seen, they
are now able to fully comprehend object permanence. Also, the child becomes capable
of deferred imitation and is able to start to solve certain types of problems
mentally without having to go through the physical actions involved. For
example, a child may throw a tantrum after seeing another child throw one an
hour ago.
Infant's Language Development
Infants learn
language by taking in information through their senses of hearing and sight as
they learn to process the meanings behind those sights and sounds.
Interestingly, newborns assimilate information immediately and are
interested in their surroundings. Few days after birth, infant recognizes
familiar faces, voices, and smells and prefers them to unfamiliar ones. Correspondingly,
infants channel their energy into seeing and hearing and their
first step in communication is by having eye contact with their
mothers, which termed as gaze coupling.
At birth, crying is an infant’s
primary means of communication. During the second month, infants begin cooing,
that is producing “oohs” and “ahs” sounds. Between 6 and 9 months, infants
start babbling which is the first vocalizing that sounds like human speech such
as “dada”. At 10 months, infants can imitate sounds by older people. An
infant’s first word usually is spoken between the ages of 11 and 13 months. By
about 18 months of age, children may be producing up to 50 words. The infant’s
vocabulary may increase to more than 300 words at about 18 to 22 months of age.
No comments:
Post a Comment